Research Article
Print
Research Article
Aliens eating aliens: an introduced amphipod as a potential prey of an invasive rocky shore crab in laboratory experiments
expand article infoNicolas Spilmont§, Laurent Seuront|
‡ Univ. Lille, Lille, France
§ Griffith University, Gold Coast Campus, Australia
| Tokyo University of Marine Science and Technology, Tokyo, Japan
¶ Rhodes University, Grahamstown, South Africa
Open Access

Abstract

Behavioural interactions between introduced predators and introduced prey are still largely underestimated. The present work takes advantage of the co-occurrence of two introduced species, the Asian shore crab Hemigrapsus sanguineus and the amphipod Ptilohyale littoralis, respectively first recorded on rocky shores along the French coast of the eastern English Channel in 2005 and 2016. In this context, the predation by male and female H. sanguineus on P. littoralis was examined under controlled laboratory conditions, by presenting either juveniles of the blue mussel Mytilus edulis or adult P. littoralis to H. sanguineus. We subsequently assessed the potential prey preference of the Asian shore crab for P. littoralis and M. edulis by presenting the two prey items simultaneously in the same proportion. In the absence of choice, male H. sanguineus preyed significantly more on M. edulis than P. littoralis. In contrast, females preyed significantly less on M. edulis than P. littoralis; however, male and female H. sanguineus consumed similar numbers of P. littoralis. When choice was possible between P. littoralis and M. edulis, the crab did not exhibit preference stricto sensu for any type of prey. These results suggest that the Asian shore crab cannot be considered as a naive predator when confronted to a newly introduced prey. Our results also suggest that the amphipod P. littoralis did not exhibit any effective antipredator response towards the crab. These observations nevertheless warrant further work on the effects of abiotic factors (e.g. temperature) as well as other biotic interactions (e.g. presence of other prey or predators for H. sanguineus) may have on the observed prey-predator interactions between H. sanguineus and M. edulis and P. littoralis.

Key words

Asian shore crab, Hemigrapsus sanguineus, amphipod, Ptilohyale littoralis, predation, behaviour

Introduction

Behaviour is one of the most critical components in the success of introduced species to become invasive (Holway and Suarez 1999; Chapple et al. 2012). More specifically, behavioural flexibility is thought to facilitate the invasion process, particularly during its early stages (Wright et al. 2010). In the last two decades, numerous studies have documented the importance of behavioural traits in the invasion success of both invertebrates and vertebrates in both terrestrial and aquatic ecosystems; see Chapple et al. (2012) and Chapple and Wong (2016) for reviews. In aquatic ecosystems, crustaceans are considered as one of the most successful groups of invasive species (Hänfling et al. 2011), and both their feeding behaviour and the nature of the subsequent predator-prey interactions appear particularly relevant to their success (Weis 2010, 2016). The predatory behaviour of invasive predators is well documented (Sih et al. 2010a; Grosholz and Wells 2016), and this holds for crustaceans (Weis 2010, 2016). In contrast, the role behaviour may have in mediating the success of invasive prey species is still a relatively untapped area of research, despite evidence that the role of behaviour in the success of invasive prey may be more important than the behaviour of native predators (Pintor and Byers 2015; Mennen and Laskowski 2018; Papacostas and Freestone 2019).

Based on the lack of co-evolution history between introduced species (either predator or prey) and their native counterparts, a range of evolutionary hypotheses has been introduced to describe novel predator-prey interactions; see e.g. Grosholz and Wells (2016) for a review. These hypotheses are essentially based on the dichotomy between the preference of both native and non-native predators towards evolutionary familiar or unfamiliar prey. Native predators may consume more of a native prey species than an invasive one, because they fail to recognize an invader (Enemy Release Hypothesis; Torchin and Mitchell 2004) and/or they are unable to overcome their defense (Novel Defense Hypothesis; Elton 1958) or because of ineffective antipredator behaviour by naive prey (Naive Prey Hypothesis; Sih et al. 2010a). Invasive predators may also favour evolutionarily familiar invasive prey species over native or unfamiliar invasive prey species (Facilitation Between Invaders Hypothesis; Grosholz and Wells 2016). Alternatively, native predators may prefer evolutionary novel non-native prey over familiar ones (Biotic Resistance Hypothesis; Elton 1958), and invasive predators may consume more of an evolutionarily novel prey (either native of from another non-native region) than evolutionary familiar ones, hence be at competitive advantage (Novel Weapons Hypothesis; Callaway and Ridenour 2004). Other hypotheses related to the Optimal Foraging Theory (Pyke 1984) and Optimal Diet Theory (Sih and Christensen 2001) respectively state that a predator consumes the prey that is the easiest to capture and the most profitable, regardless of evolutionary or historical familiarity.

The nature of the abovementioned predator-prey interactions is, however, noticeably both species- and location-dependent, and may also vary along the invasion process due to a potential loss of naiveté (Grosholz and Wells 2016). It is, however, essential to the understanding of the impact of invasions on ecosystem structure and function to examine the behaviour of invasive species in their different introduction areas, since behavioural flexibility may lead a given species to express different behavioural variants in distinct ecological conditions (Wright et al. 2010). In this context, the present work takes advantage of the recently documented (Spilmont et al. 2018) co-occurrence of the Asian shore crab Hemigrapsus sanguineus and the North American amphipod Ptilohyale littoralis along the French coast of the eastern English Channel. Studies on predation and prey selection by the Asian shore crab showed that this species is omnivorous, though it exhibits a preference towards animal prey (Brousseau and Baglivo 2005). In particular, juveniles of the blue mussel Mytilus edulis are actively selected (Bouwmeester et al. 2020), including over other invertebrates both in field and laboratory experiments (Lohrer and Whitlatch 2002). In addition, amphipods are known to be a recurrent component of the diet of H. sanguineus (McDermott 1999; Lohrer et al. 2000; Blasi and O’Connor 2016). The present study assesses, based on experiments run under controlled laboratory conditions, (i) if Hemigrapsus sanguineus is actually able to prey on Ptilohyale littoralis from the same introduced area, and (ii) if H. sanguineus exhibits a preference between P. littoralis and the blue mussel Mytilus edulis, known as one of its preferred prey.

Methods

Study species

Hemigrapsus sanguineus (De Haan, 1835) is a native species of the North-West Pacific coast which has been reported along the North Atlantic European coast in the late 1990’s (Breton et al. 2002) and up north on the Opal Coast circa 2005 (Dauvin et al. 2009). Populations now extend from Normandy up to the coast of Sweden (Gothland et al. 2013; Jungblut et al. 2017) and the species has been recorded in Great Britain (Seeley et al. 2015). The biology of H. sanguineus outside its native range is well documented, especially in North America (see Epifanio 2013 for a review) where it has first been recorded in New Jersey in the late 1980’s (Williams and McDermott 1990). In contrast, far less is known about European H. sanguineus, especially regarding its behaviour (Spilmont et al. 2015).

The North American amphipod Ptilohyale littoralis (Stimpson, 1853) was first reported in Europe in the Port of Rotterdam in 2009, where it is believed to have been introduced through ballast water and/or hull fouling, and subsequently at the mouth of the Westerschelde estuary and in Yerseke (Faasse 2014). P. littoralis has recently been recorded at a site colonized by H. sanguineus in Wimereux, France (Spilmont et al. 2018), where it was consistently found within beds of the common blue mussel Mytilus edulis.

Animal collection and maintenance

Animals were collected during early spring (March-April 2016) in the mid-intertidal zone of the rocky reef “Fort de Croy” located in Wimereux, France (50°45.766'N, 1°35.962'E). Adults of H. sanguineus were collected manually, brought back to the nearby laboratory (LOG, Station Marine de Wimereux), sex determined, and males and non-ovigerous females were kept in separate tanks (52 cm × 46 cm × 35 cm) containing rocks from the sampling site, with running natural seawater at in situ temperature (11 °C) under a natural day/night cycle. Food (commercial fish pellets) was provided ad libitum but, to standardize hunger levels, crabs were starved for 24 h prior to experiments.

During the low tide preceding each experiment, sediment was collected at the same sampling site. Following Blasi & O’Connor (2016), sediment containing amphipods was placed into a bucket filled with seawater and swirled by hand to cause amphipods to swim up into the water. The water was then sieved (1 mm mesh-size) and visually inspected for the presence of Ptilohyale littoralis. Living individuals were carefully collected with forceps and checked under binoculars. Undamaged large individuals of similar sizes (10–12 mm length) were then kept in the laboratory in PVC cylinders (10 cm in diameter) with mesh bottom, suspended in aquaria with running natural seawater. Juvenile mussels (Mytilus edulis; length in the range 5–10 mm) recovered from the same sediment samples were kept in separate aquaria (same conditions as H. sanguineus). All living animals were returned to the sampling location after completion of the experiments.

Experimental design

For all predation trials, non-moulting adult H. sanguineus (carapace width CW = 21.02 ± 2.24 mm, mean ± SD, N = 100) with intact chelae were individually isolated in small plastic aquaria (16.0 cm × 9.5 cm × 10.5 cm) without sediment (to easily retrieve non-consumed amphipods), closed with plastic lids and immersed in larger glass aquaria (90 cm × 50 cm × 30 cm) with running natural seawater. A shelter was offered in each experimental aquarium in the form of a dark gray PVC pipe (6.5 cm long, 4 cm in diameter) longitudinally cut in half. Since H. sanguineus preferentially feeds at night due to its photophobic behaviour (Spilmont et al. 2015), all experiments were conducted for 12 h during nighttime (i.e. the experiments started at dusk, were run overnight, and stopped early in the morning). At the end of each experiment, crabs were removed and the number of remaining prey (amphipods and/or mussels) in each aquarium was counted. No significant differences in individuals CW were found (Wilcoxon-Mann-Whitney U-test, P > 0.05) between males and females within each treatment and between treatments for each sex; as such, potential differences observed between sexes and treatments could not be attributed to a size effect. To ensure the independence of the trials, each experimental individual was used only once and, between each experiment, aquaria and PVC shelter were washed with seawater, rinsed with 95% ethanol, then with distilled water (3 times) and allowed to dry until the next experiment.

Predation trials and prey preference

Attempts to provide a definition to food/prey ‘preference’ have a long-lasting history (see e.g. Rapport and Turner 1970; Johnson 1980). The related definition, associated experimental setups and data analysis have been more recently debated (Underwood et al. 2004; Underwood and Clarke 2005, 2006, 2007; Manly 2006; Taplin 2007). However, to date, no consensus has arisen which critically stresses the need to be very specific about the definition of ‘preference’ and the choice of experimental setups (Underwood and Clarke 2006).

Here, we specifically considered preference stricto sensu as a behaviour implying an active choice between distinct food items. To be objectively and quantitatively inferred, this definition requires the comparison of food consumption when only one prey species is available with consumption when several prey species are available, as initially defined by Rapport and Turner (1970) and Johnson (1980). Specifically, our experimental design was setup following Underwood et al. (2004) and Underwood and Clarke (2005). This approach has previously been used to determine food preference in crabs (Laitano et al. 2013) and is typically applied in food preference experiments (Astudillo et al. 2018). The design is based on a two-stage experiment. First, prey items are presented one type at a time to the predator; the results of these ‘no-choice experiments’ (or in Stage 1) provide the consumption rate of each type of prey (number of prey eaten per predator). In turn, in ‘choice experiments’ (Stage 2), two distinct prey items are presented simultaneously in the exact same proportion. Note that differences in consumption rates do not constitute preference stricto sensu but are rather the result of differences in detectability, catchability, handling time and satiety power in prey. As a consequence, preference by the predator towards a specific type of prey can only be tested by comparing the predicted proportions of prey items consumed in the choice experiments under the hypothesis of no preference with the actual proportions observed in Stage 2. This also implies that, in the framework of this definition of preference, the preferred prey is not necessarily the most consumed.

It has also been suggested that, in choice experiments, not only the proportions but also the order in which prey are consumed is important to determine preference (Taplin 2007). Since our experimental setup did not allow such a monitoring, we conducted a supplementary experiment, hereafter called Stage 2bis, to confirm the results obtained in Stage 2. In the Stage 2bis experiment, prey were presented in unbalanced proportions, i.e. each crab was offered one individual of a type of prey and 5 individuals of the other type. If one type of prey is actually preferred over the other one, it would be consumed first, irrespectively of its relative abundance (Taplin 2007), i.e. it would always be consumed when offered alone vs. 5 items of the other prey.

The prey tested in the present study were either P. littoralis or M. edulis (5 individuals offered to each crab: no-choice experiments) or a mixture of both (5 individuals of each prey offered to each crab: choice experiments); in Stage 2bis, 1 individual of one type and 5 individuals of the other were offered to each crab. A batch of 10 H. sanguineus individuals was tested during each experimental night; sexes were randomly selected for each batch to attain a total of 15 males and 15 females for each type of predation trial (Table 1). After each experiment, we counted the number of prey that were eaten. Furthermore, to assess the natural mortality of P. littoralis, control aquaria were run with only 5 P. littoralis present, with the same setup as described above. No mortality was recorded and all 5 amphipods were always recovered at the end of the 10 controls run.

Table 1.

Experimental treatments used to test for predation of female and male H. sanguineus on Mytilus edulis and Ptilohyale littoralis in no-choice experiments (Stage 1) and choice experiments (Stage 2 and Stage 2bis). Control experiments were run to test on the mortality of P. littoralis.

Experiment Prey Predator replicates (N)
M. edulis P. littoralis female H. sanguineus male H. sanguineus
Control 0 5 0 0 10
Stage 1 5 0 0 1 15
5 0 1 0 15
0 5 0 1 15
0 5 1 0 15
Stage 2 5 5 0 1 15
5 5 1 0 15
Stage 2bis 1 5 0 1 15
1 5 1 0 15
5 1 0 1 15
5 1 1 0 15

Statistical analysis

To compare results obtained from the no-choice experiments, we used the non-parametric Wilcoxon-Mann-Whitney U test (WMW test hereafter).

For preference tests, we used the procedure proposed by Underwood and Clarke (2005). This procedure, that takes into account potential sampling errors when samples are small, uses a Chi-square test (χ2) where the expected numbers on the basis of no preference are obtained through maximum likelihood equations calculated after results obtained from no-choice (Stage 1) and choice experiments (Stage 2; section 2.4 and Appendix A.3 in Underwood and Clarke 2005). Since we considered that preference per se can only be determined by comparing predicted values for choice experiments under the hypothesis of no preference with observed values in these experiments, χ2 tests were computed comparing observed numbers with expected numbers only for Stage 2. A global χ2 test was therefore computed, by pooling data obtained from all replicates, as implemented by Laitano et al. (2013). To examine the potential inter-individual variation (Manly 2006; Taplin 2007), random pairs of experimental units of predation on Stage 1 and Stage 2 were chosen for comparison (Hall-Scharf and Stallings 2014; Astudillo et al. 2018); 15 of these random pairs were examined for male and female H. sanguineus by calculating individual χ2 tests.

Results

In no-choice experiments (Stage 1; Fig. 1A), male H. sanguineus exhibited a higher consumption rate on M. edulis (prey consumed per individual: 3.7 ± 1.3 prey ind-1; mean ± standard deviation) than on P. littoralis (2.1 ± 1.5 prey ind-1; WMW test: P = 0.004). The reverse situation was observed for females (WMW test: P = 2.1 10-4) with a higher consumption of amphipods (2.5 ± 1.6 prey ind-1) than mussels (0.3 ± 0.8 prey ind-1).

Figure 1. 

Number (mean ± S.D., N = 15 for each bar) of prey (Mytilus edulis, grey bars; Ptilohyale littoralis, white bars) consumed by male and female Hemigrapsus sanguineus over 12 h (nighttime) during no-choice experiments (A: Stage 1, prey presented one type at a time) and choice experiments (B: Stage, two types of prey presented simultaneously).

The consumption rate of M. edulis significantly differed between male and female H. sanguineus (WMW test: P = 4.4 10-6). Specifically, twelve females did not prey on M. edulis and the maximum number of mussels eaten by a single individual was 3. In sharp contrast, all males consumed at least one mussel and five of them did prey on the 5 individuals presented. The consumption rate of P. littoralis was non-significantly different between males and females (WMW test: P = 0.445).

The results of the choice experiments (Stage 2; Fig. 1B) showed that the consumption rate for each type of prey did not significantly differ from the no-choice experiment (WMW tests, P > 0.148). In addition, the lack of significant difference between the expected and observed consumption rates indicates the absence of preference stricto sensu for any type of prey (Table 2), either for males (Chi-square test: χ21 = 0.76, P = 0.385) or for females (Chi-square test: χ21 = 0.01, P = 0.904). Thirteen out of the 15 individual χ2 tests comparisons for males showed no significant differences, and all 15 tests were not significant for females (Table 2); this confirmed that the observed consumption rates were not influenced by inter-individual variability. These results were further supported by those from the complementary choice experiment (Stage 2bis, i.e. prey presented in unbalanced proportions, Fig. 2) which showed than neither M. edulis nor P. littoralis was systematically consumed when a single individual was offered (none of the prey was systematically consumed first), i.e. none of the prey is preferred stricto sensu following Taplin (2007).

Table 2.

Consumed individuals of each type of prey (Mytilus edulis, M. e. and Ptilohyale littoralis, P. l.) by adult female and male Hemigrapsus sanguineus observed (Obs) during no-choice experiments (Stage 1) and choice experiments (Stage 2). Expected values (Exp) were obtained from the equations of maximal likelihood given by Underwood and Clarke (2005). Chi-square tests were used to evaluate differences between observed and expected values in Stage 2. For the global χ2 test, the total number of each type of prey offered equals 75 both in Stage 1 and Stage 2 (5 prey × 15 replicates). For individual χ2 tests, the total number of each prey offered was 5, both in Stage 1 and Stage 2.

Males Stage 1 Stage 2 χ2 P value
M. e. Obs P. l. Obs M. e. Obs P. l. Obs M. e. Exp P. l. Exp
Global 55 32 47 20 43.6 23.4 0.76 0.385
1 3 4 2 3 2.1 2.8 0.01 0.916
2 5 3 5 2 4.5 2.5 0.16 0.693
3 4 4 3 2 2.6 2.4 0.16 0.691
4 3 2 5 3 4.9 3.1 0.01 0.937
5 3 2 3 0 2.1 0.9 1.24 0.266
6 4 3 5 1 3.8 1 0.99 0.318
7 5 0 3 0 3 0 0 1
8 5 1 1 3 2.8 1.2 4.03 0.04
9 5 1 1 4 3.4 1.6 5.43 0.02
10 2 5 3 2 3.3 1.7 1.46 0.226
11 1 0 3 0 3 0 0 1
12 4 3 5 0 3.4 1.6 2.40 0.121
13 4 1 3 0 2.6 0.4 0.47 0.491
14 2 2 5 0 3.7 1.3 1.78 0.182
15 5 1 4 0 3.6 0.4 0.49 0.483
Females Stage 1 Stage 2 χ2 P value
M. e. Obs P. l. Obs M. e. Obs P. l. Obs M. e. Exp P. l. Exp
Global 5 38 4 34 0.2 33.8 0.01 0.904
1 0 1 0 5 0 5 0 1
2 1 3 1 4 1 4 0 1
3 0 3 0 3 0 3 0 1
4 0 3 0 1 0 1 0 1
5 0 3 0 1 0 1 0 1
6 3 4 0 2 1 1 2.2 0.136
7 0 3 0 2 0 2 0 1
8 0 5 0 2 0 2 0 1
9 0 4 0 4 0 4 0 1
10 0 3 0 5 0 5 0 1
11 1 2 0 0 0 0 0 1
12 0 4 1 2 0.4 2.6 1.2 0.277
13 0 3 1 0 0.2 0.8 3.8 0.05
14 1 4 1 1 0.5 1.5 0.6 0.434
15 1 2 0 2 0.4 1.6 0.6 0.457
Figure 2. 

Proportion of trials when the single prey was eaten (%) during the choice experiments with unbalanced proportions of prey (Stage 2 bis; white bars: 1 mussel and 5 amphipods presented simultaneously; grey bars: 5 mussels and 1 amphipod presented simultaneously).

Discussion

The present study provides evidence, under laboratory-controlled conditions, for the existence of predator-prey interactions between two introduced crustaceans, the Asian shore crab Hemigrapsus sanguineus and the North American amphipod Ptilohyale littoralis, co-occurring outside their distinct native ranges in the intertidal zone of the eastern English Channel. Specifically, in no-choice experiments, both male and female H. sanguineus actively selected both M. edulis and P. littoralis, with females exhibiting a significantly lower consumption rate (i) on M. edulis than males, but not on P. littoralis, and (ii) on the native M. edulis than on the non-native P. littoralis. These results are consistent with previous studies showing a stronger predation pressure of males H. sanguineus on M. edulis than females due to differences in claw morphology and strength (Brousseau et al. 2001; Bourdeau and O’Connor 2003), and similar predation pressure on the amphipod Hyale plumulosa from male and female H. sanguineus (Blasi and O’Connor, 2016). Our observations are consistent with studies demonstrating the ability of crabs of the genus Hemigrapsus to feed on amphipods in laboratory experiments (McDermott 1999; Griffen and Byers 2006; Cornelius et al. 2021; Bleile and Thieltges 2021), with males exerting a higher predation pressure on mussels than amphipods (Cornelius et al. 2021; Bleile and Thieltges 2021), contrary to females (Cornelius et al. 2021).

In two-prey choice experiments, H. sanguineus consistently did not exhibit any preference stricto sensu for either native or non-native prey though significant differences in the consumption rates of both types of prey were observed. This result seems to diverge from previous evidence that the Asian shore crab preferred mussels over other animal prey (Tyrrell and Harris 2000; Brousseau and Baglivo 2005), including in experiments with amphipods (Bleile and Thieltges 2021). However, the outcome from Bleile and Thieltges (2021) regarding preference are not directly comparable to the present study due to experiments restricted to males, choice experiments conducted with 4 types of prey and differences in the definition of “preference” itself and the subsequent data analysis. We, however, showed that male H. sanguineus actually consumed more M. edulis individuals than P. littoralis, which is consistent with most studies defining preference as a higher consumption rate. Terminological considerations aside, this recurrent observation in laboratory experiments on H. sanguineus is of prime importance regarding its ecological impact. Though not always preferred stricto sensu, juvenile mussels may constitute a large part of the crab’s diet which may ultimately have a critical influence on the composition of natural benthic intertidal communities relying on M. edulis as engineer species.

We, however, unambiguously showed that H. sanguineus is able to detect and consume P. littoralis although the latter was putatively introduced a decade later (i.e. circa 2016; Spilmont et al. 2018) than the former (i.e. circa 2005; Dauvin et al. 2009) at our study site. This suggests that the Asian shore crab cannot be considered as a naive predator when confronted to a newly introduced amphipod prey. This apparent lack of naiveté is consistent with the ability of H. sanguineus to rapidly sense, pursue and consume different amphipod preys (McDermott 1999). Despite a lack of co-evolutionary history and historical familiarity between H. sanguineus and P. littoralis in their introduced eastern English Channel range, handling skills learned by H. sanguineus either in their native or non-native ranges in the presence of similar prey (e.g. Apohyale prevostii; Spilmont et al. 2018) could have been transferred to novel prey, as shown in the shore crab Carcinus maenas (Hughes and O’Brien 2001). Note that the indigenous M. edulis was not considered as a naive prey towards H. sanguineus; indeed, the present experiments were conducted about 10 years after the introduction of H. sanguineus at our study site and it can be considered that M. edulis is now able to detect the presence of the Asian shore crab’s cues, as recently demonstrated by Uguen et al. (2022) for mussels and H. sanguineus from the same study site.

Since H. sanguineus did not exhibit any preference stricto sensu for either the indigenous (M. edulis) or the introduced (P. littoralis) prey, none of the evolutionary hypotheses of novel prey-predator interactions (see Grosholz and Wells 2016) applies to H. sanguineus in its introduced range along the French coast of the eastern English Channel. Instead, following Grosholz and Wells (2016), H. sanguineus would use a combination of optimal foraging strategy and optimal diet strategy and thus consume the prey that is the easiest to catch and the most profitable. However, the optimal diet theory is usually not supported when predators attack motile prey (Sih and Christensen 2001). Indeed, consuming a motile small amphipod would unlikely maximize the net energy gain per unit time foraging compared to the consumption of the larger non- or slow-moving mussel. It is actually much more probable that H. sanguineus is an opportunistic predator and that its consumption of P. littoralis mainly depends on their encounter rate. This hypothesis in consistent with the fact that invasive prey usually exhibit various degrees of activity, sheltering and exploratory behaviours (Chapple and Wong 2016) which, when the species is considered as a prey, would either mitigate or enhance the predation pressure. This was demonstrated in studies with the invasive freshwater amphipod Dikerogammarus villosus exhibiting less exploratory behaviour and greater sheltering than native species, which made the former less vulnerable to fish predation (Kobak et al. 2014; Mennen and Laskowski 2018). In our case, the degree of activity of P. littoralis could be estimated only by complementary experiments with continuous monitoring.

The fact that a motile prey such as P. littoralis was consumed in high proportion, especially by female H. sanguineus, implicitly suggests, that P. littoralis either did not exhibit any, or expressed inappropriate/uneffective, antipredator response, which would support the ‘naive prey’ hypothesis (Banks and Dickman 2007; Sih et al. 2010b). Though, to our knowledge, anti-predator responses have never been investigated in Hyalid amphipods, this has been demonstrated in other families (e.g. Mennen and Laskowski 2018). The consumption rates we recorded on P. littoralis are consistent with those observed by Blasi and O’Connor (2016) – note that Hyale plumulosa’s accepted name is actually Ptilohyale plumulosa (WoRMS 2020), which has been categorized as P. littoralis for the Atlantic ; see pp. 102–103 in Bousfield and Hendrycks 2002 and p. 51 in Lo Brutto and Iaciofano 2018 – in experimental enclosures without any shelter (consumption of ca. 60% of prey available). Our estimates are, however, clearly higher than the consumption rates they recorded with a refuge (sediment and rocks; consumption of ca. 15%). Despite differences in the experimental setup, these discrepancies likely reflect different behaviours and/or antipredator responses of P. littoralis between native and invaded ranges. For the eastern English Channel area, similar laboratory observations using native amphipods, such as the sibling species Apohyale prevostii or the Melitidae Melita palmata would help to more thoroughly elucidate interactions between H. sanguineus and amphipods by comparing native and introduced prey.

Our results are relevant in an experimental context and, though controlled experiments remain a stepping stone in the understanding of prey-predator interactions, the extrapolation of laboratory observations to field events remains uncertain (Brousseau and Baglivo 2005). Our experiments represented idealized foraging conditions due to the small size of the experimental container that minimized search time of the predatory crab and overestimate ingestion under natural conditions (Lohrer and Whitlach 2002; Epifanio 2013). They were also conducted at night when H. sanguineus is known to be more actively feeding (Spilmont et al. 2015) and without sediment in the containers which added to the optimal foraging conditions (Blasi and O’Connor 2016). Feeding patterns of the Asian shore crab on mussels and amphipods have been shown to be different in the field and in the laboratory (Brousseau et al. 2014; Blasi and O’Connor 2016); possible explanations include limited foraging opportunities due to abiotic factors (diurnal cycle, wave action, etc), increased prey choice and variable densities.

Though being part of the diet of the Asian shore crab in both its native and north-American invaded ranges, amphipods usually constitute only a small proportion of the overall food use (Lohrer et al. 2000; Griffen et al. 2008). However, a potential impact of the sibling H. takanoi on natural amphipod populations has recently been underlined (Cornelius et al. 2021) and our results suggest that the predatory pressure on P. littoralis would mainly be exerted by female H. sanguineus when several prey are available. At our study site, H. sanguineus was found concomitantly with P. littoralis, but also with a range of potential other prey (e.g. the amphipods Apohyale prevostii and Melita palmata, the annelid Eulalia clavigera and the gastropod Littorina littorea), as well as the potentially competitive green crab Carcinus maenas (Spilmont et al. 2018). Furthermore, the Asian shore crab is itself a potential prey for fish (Heinonen and Auster 2012; Savaria and O’Connor 2013), birds (Dumoulin and van Outryve 2009) and other crab species (Papacostas and Freestone 2019), against which H. sanguineus could also behave as a naive prey. The potential benefit for the crab to forage on naive prey could therefore be dampened by its own naiveté (Papacostas and Freestone 2019). As long as these abiotic factors and consumptive and non-consumptive biotic interactions are not taken into account, neither the ecological role of H. sanguineus, which as a large benthic decapod could play a role in structuring communities (Boudreau and Worm 2012), nor the potential synergistic interaction between the two introduce species, which could accelerate impacts on native communities (‘invasional meltdown’: Simberloff and Von Holle 1999), can be elucidated.

Funding

This work is a contribution to the CPER research project CLIMIBIO. The authors thank the French Ministère de l’Enseignement Supérieur et de la Recherche, the Hauts de France Région and the European Funds for Regional Economical Development for their financial support for this project. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Auhthors’ Contribution

N.S.: research conceptualization, sample design and methodology, investigation and data collection, data analysis and interpretation, original draft; writing

L.S.: research conceptualization, sample design and methodology, data interpretation, original draft; review & editing

Acknowledgements

The authors thank the organizing committee of the ICAIS 2022 conference and especially H. Verreycken for handling the ICAIS 2022 species issue of Aquatic Invasions as lead guest editor. We also thank anonymous reviewers for their constructive comments and suggestions which improved this paper.

References

  • Blasi JC, O’Connor NJ (2016) Amphipods as potential prey of the Asian shore crab Hemigrapsus sanguineus: Laboratory and field experiments. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 474: 18–22. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jembe.2015.09.011
  • Bleile N, Thieltges DW (2021) Prey preferences of invasive (Hemigrapsus sanguineus, H. takanoi) and native (Carcinus maenas) intertidal crabs in the Eurpoean Wadden Sea. Journal of the Marine Biological Association of the United Kingdom 101: 811–817. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0025315421000655
  • Boudreau SA, Worm B (2012) Ecological role of large benthic decapods in marine ecosystems: a review. Marine Ecology Progress Series 469: 195–213. https://doi.org/10.3354/meps09862
  • Bousfield EL, Hendrycks EA (2002) The talitroidean amphipod family Hyalidae revised, with emphasis on the North Pacific fauna: systematics and distribution ecology. Amphipacifica 3: 17–134.
  • Bouwmeester MM, Waser AM, ven der Meer J, Thieltges DW (2020) Prey size selection in invasive (Hemigrapsus sanguineus and H. takanoi) compared with native (Carcinus maenas) marine crabs. Journal of the Marine Biological Association of the United Kingdom 100: 73–77. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0025315419000985
  • Breton G, Faasse M, Noël P, Vincent T (2002) A new alien crab in Europe: Hemigrapsus sanguineus (Decapoda: Brachyura: Grapsidae). Journal of Crustacean Biology 22: 184–189. https://doi.org/10.1163/20021975-99990221
  • Brousseau DJ, Baglivo JA (2005) Laboratory investigations of food selection by the Asian shore crab, Hemigrapsus sanguineus: algal versus animal preference. Journal of Crustacean Biology 25: 130–134. https://doi.org/10.1651/C-2530
  • Brousseau DJ, Filipowicz A, Baglivo JA (2001) Laboratory investigations of the effects of predator sex and size on prey selection by the Asian crab, Hemigrapsus sanguineus. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 262: 199–210. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-0981(01)00290-8
  • Brousseau DJ, Goldberg R, Garza C (2014) Impact of predation by the invasive crab Hemigrapsus sanguineus on survival of juvenile blue mussels in western Long Island Sound. Northeastern Naturalist 21: 119–133. https://doi.org/10.1656/045.021.0110
  • Chapple DG, Simmonds SM, Wong BBM (2012) Can behavioral and personality traits influence the success of unintentional species introductions. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 27: 57–64. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2011.09.010
  • Chapple DG, Wong BBM (2016) The role of behavioural variation across different stages of the introduction process. In: Weis JS, Sol D (Eds) Biological invasions and animal behaviour, Cambridge University Press, New York, 7–25. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139939492.003
  • Cornelius A, Wagner K, Buschbaum C (2021) Prey preferences, consumption rates and predation effects of Asian shore crabs (Hemigrapsus takanoi) in comparison to native shore crabs (Carcinus maenas) in northwestern Europe. Marine Biodiversity 51: 75. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12526-021-01207-7
  • Dauvin J-C, Tous Rius A, Ruellet T (2009) Recent expansion of two invasive crabs species Hemigrapsus sanguineus (de Haan, 1835) and H. takanoi Asakura and Watanabe 2005 along the Opal Coast, France. Aquatic Invasions 4: 451–465. https://doi.org/10.3391/ai.2009.4.3.3
  • Dumoulin E, van Outryve R (2009) Predatie door vogels van penseelkrabbetjes (Hemigrapsus spp.) en Japanse oester (Crassostrea gigas): waarnemingen aan scholeksters (Haematopus ostralegus), wulpen (Numenius arquata), regenwulpen (Numenius phaeopus), steenlopers (Arenaria interpres) en kauwen (Corvus monedula) in de Ijzermonding te Nieuwpoort. De Strandvlo 29: 72–104.
  • Gothland M, Dauvin J-C, Denis L, Jobert S, Ovaert J, Pezy J-P, Spilmont N (2013) Additional and distribution (2011–2012) of Hemigrapsus sanguineus (De Haan, 1835) along the French coast of the English Channel. Management of Biological Invasions 4: 305–315. https://doi.org/10.3391/mbi.2013.4.4.05
  • Griffen BD, Guy T, Buck JC (2008) Inhibition between invasives: a newly introduced predator moderates the impacts of a previously established invasive predator. Journal of Animal Ecology 77: 32–40. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2656.2007.01304.x
  • Grosholz ED, Wells EH (2016) Evolutionary novelty and the behaviour of introduced predators. In: Weis JS, Sol D (Eds) Biological invasions and animal behaviour, Cambridge University Press, New York, 199–217. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139939492.013
  • Hall-Scharf BJ, Stallings CD (2014) Experimental test of preference by a generalist piscivore on morphologically- and behaviorally- different prey. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 460: 193–196. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jembe.2014.07.006
  • Heinonen KB, Auster PJ (2012) Prey selection in crustacean-eating fishes following the invasion of the Asian shore crab Hemigrapsus sanguineus in a marine temperate community. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 413: 177–183. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jembe.2011.12.011
  • Johnson DH (1980) The comparison of usage and availability measurements for evaluations resource preference. Ecology 61: 65–71. https://doi.org/10.2307/1937156
  • Jungblut S, Beerman J, Boos K, Soborowski R, Hagen W (2017) Population development of the invasive crab Hemigrapsus sanguineus (De Haan, 1853) and its potential competitor Carcinus maenas (Linnaeus, 1758) at Helgoland (North Sea) between 2009 and 2014. Aquatic Invasions 12: 85–96. https://doi.org/10.3391/ai.2017.12.1.09
  • Kobak J, Jermacz Ł, Płąchocki D (2014) Effectiveness of zebra mussels to act as shelters from fish predators differs between native and invasive amphipod prey. Aquatic Ecology 48: 397–408. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10452-014-9492-1
  • Lo Brutto S, Iaciofano D (2018) A taxonomic revision helps to clarify differences between the Atlantic invasive Ptilohyale littoralis and the Mediterranean Parahyale plumicornis (Crustacea, Amphipoda). ZooKeys 754: 47–62. https://doi.org/10.3897/zookeys.754.22884
  • Lohrer AM, Whitlatch RB (2002) Relative impacts of two exotic brachyuran species on blue mussel populations in Long Island Sound. Marine Ecology Progress Series 227: 135–144. https://doi.org/10.3354/meps227135
  • Lohrer AM, Whitlach RB, Wada K, Fukui Y (2000) Home and away: comparisons of resource utilization by a marine species in native and invaded habitats. Biological Invasions 2: 41–57. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1010069327402
  • McDermott JJ (1999) The western Pacific brachyuran Hemigrapsus sanguineus (Grapsidae) in its new habitat along the Atlantic coast of the United States: feeding, cheliped morphology and growth. In: Schram FR, von Vaupel Klein JC (Eds) Crustaceans and the biodiversity crisis, Proceedings of the fourth International Crustacean Congress, Brill, Leiden, 425–444.
  • Pintor LM, Byers JE (2015) Individual variation in predator behavior and demographics affects consumption of non-native prey. Behavioral Ecology 26: 797–804. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arv013
  • Savaria MC, O’Connor NJ (2013) Predation of the non-native Asian shore crab Hemigrapsus sanguineus by a native fish species, the cunner (Tautogolabrus adspersus). Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 449: 335–339. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jembe.2013.10.003
  • Seeley B, Sewell J, Clark PF (2015) First GB records of the invasive Asian shore crab, Hemigrapsus sanguineus from Glamorgan, Wales and Kent, England. Marine Biodiversity Records 8: e102. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1755267215000809
  • Sih A, Bolnick DI, Luttbeg B, Orrock JL, Peacor SD, Pintor LM, Preissier E, Rehage JS, Vonesh J (2010a) Predator-prey naïveté, antipredator behavior, and the ecology of predator invasion. Oikos 119: 610–621. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0706.2009.18039.x
  • Sih A, Stamps J, Yang LH, McElreath R, Ramenofsky M (2010b) Behavior as a key component of integrative biology in a human-altered world. Integrative and Comparative Biology 50: 934–944. https://doi.org/10.1093/icb/icq148
  • Spilmont N, Gothland M, Seuront L (2015) Exogenous control of the feeding activity in the invasive Asian shore crab Hemigrapsus sanguineus (De Haan, 1835). Aquatic Invasions 10: 327–332. https://doi.org/10.3391/ai.2015.10.3.07
  • Spilmont N, Hachet A, Faasse MA, Jourde J, Luczak C, Seuront L, Rolet C (2018) First records of Ptilohyale littoralis (Amphipoda: Hyalidae) and Boccardia proboscidea (Polychaeta: Spionidae) of the English Channel: habitat use and coexistence with other species. Marine Biodiversity 48: 1109–1119. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12526-016-0557-3
  • Tyrrell MC, Harris LG (2000) Potential impact of the introduced Asian shore crab, Hemigrapsus sanguineus, in Northen New England: diet, feeding preferences, and overlap with the green crab, Carcinus maenas. In: Pederson J (Ed.) Marine bioinvasions, Proceedings of the First National Conference, 208–220.
  • Uguen M, Nicastro KR, Zardi GI, Gaudron SM, Spilmont N, Akoueson F, Duflos G, Seuront L (2022) Microplastic leachates disrupt the chemotactic and chemokinetic behaviours of an ecosystem engineer (Mytilus edulis). Chemosphere 306: 135425. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chemosphere.2022.135425
  • Underwood AJ, Chapman MG, Crowe TP (2004) Identifying and understanding ecological preferences for habitat or prey. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 300: 161–187. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jembe.2003.12.006
  • Underwood AJ, Clarke KR (2005) Solving some statistical problems in analyses of experiments on choices of food and associations with habitat. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 318: 227–237. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jembe.2004.12.014
  • Underwood AJ, Clarke KR (2006) Response on a proposed method for analysing experiments on food choice. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 335: 151–153. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jembe.2006.02.014
  • Underwood AJ, Clarke KR (2007) More response on a proposed method for analysing experiments on food choice. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 344: 113–115. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jembe.2006.12.029
  • Weis JS (2016) Eat or be eaten: invasion and predation in aquatic ecosystems. In: Weis JS, Sol D (Eds) Biological invasions and animal behaviour Cambridge University Press, New York, 180–198. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139939492.012
  • Williams AB, McDermott JJ (1990) An eastern United States record for the western Indo-Pacific crab Hemigrapsus sanguineus (Crustacea: Decapoda: Grapsidae). Proceedings of the Biological Society of Washington 103: 108–109.
  • Wright TF, Eberhard JR, Hobson EA, Avery ML, Russello MA (2010) Behavioral flexibility and species invasions: the adaptative flexibility hypothesis. Ethology Ecology & Evolution 22: 393–404. https://doi.org/10.1080/03949370.2010.505580
login to comment